Those with a higher need to belong are better at sustaining friendships Quizlet

1. Boyd R, Richerson PJ, Henrich J. 2011. The cultural niche: why social learning is essential for human adaptation. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 108, 10 918–10 925. ( 10.1073/pnas.1100290108) [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

2. Harris PL. 2012. Trusting what you're told: how children learn from others. Cambridge, MA: The Belknap Press/Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]

3. Tomasello M, Melis AP, Tennie C, Wyman E, Herrmann E. 2012. Two key steps in the evolution of human cooperation: the interdependence hypothesis. Curr. Anthropol. 53, 673–692. ( 10.1086/668207) [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

4. Hay DF, Payne A, Chadwick A. 2004. Peer relations in childhood. J. Child Psychol. Psychiat. 45, 84–108. ( 10.1046/j.0021-9630.2003.00308.x) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

5. Wimmer H, Perner J. 1983. Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children's understanding of deception. Cognition 13, 103–128. ( 10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

6. Baillargeon R, He Z, Setoh P, Scott RM, Sloane S, Yang DY-J. 2013. False-belief understanding and why it matters: the social-acting hypothesis. In Navigating the social world: what infants, children, and other species can teach us (eds Banaji MR, Gelman SA), pp. 88–95. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]

7. Sebanz N, Bekkering H, Knoblich G. 2006. Joint action: bodies and minds moving together. Trends Cogn. Sci. 10, 70–76. ( 10.1016/j.tics.2005.12.009) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

8. Wohlschläger A, Gattis M, Bekkering H. 2003. Action generation and action perception in imitation: an instance of the ideomotor principle. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 358, 501–515. ( 10.1098/rstb.2002.1257) [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

9. Wang Y, Hamilton A. 2012. Social top-down response modulation (STORM): a model of the control of mimicry in social interaction. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6, 1–10. ( 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00153) [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

10. Onishi KH, Baillargeon R. 2005. Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science 308, 255–258. ( 10.1126/science.1107621) [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

11. Meltzoff AN. 1995. Understanding the intentions of others: re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Dev. Psychol. 31, 838–850. ( 10.1037/0012-1649.31.5.838) [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

12. Chevallier C, Kohls G, Troiani V, Brodkin ES, Schultz RT. 2012. The social motivation theory of autism. Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 231–239. ( 10.1016/j.tics.2012.02.007) [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

13. Tomasello M, Carpenter M, Call J, Behne T, Moll H. 2005. Understanding and sharing intentions: the origins of cultural cognition. Behav. Brain Sci. 28, 675–691. ( 10.1017/S0140525X05000129) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

14. Apperly IA, Butterfill SA. 2009. Do humans have two systems to track beliefs and belief-like states? Psychol. Rev. 116, 953–970. ( 10.1037/a0016923) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

15. Heyes CM. 2013. What can imitation do for cooperation? In Cooperation and its evolution (eds Stereiny K, Joyce R, Calcott B, Fraser B), pp. 313–333. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]

16. Meltzoff AN, Moore MK. 1997. Explaining facial imitation: a theoretical model. Early Dev. Parenting 6, 179–192. ( 10.1002/(SICI)1099-0917(199709/12)6:3/4<179::AID-EDP157>3.0.CO;2-R) [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

17. Nielsen M. 2009. The imitative behavior of children and chimpanzees: a window on the transmission of cultural traditions. Rev. Primatol. 1, 254 ( 10.4000/primatologie.254) [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

18. Over H, Carpenter M. 2012. Putting the social into social learning: explaining both selectivity and fidelity in children's copying behavior. J. Comp. Psychol. 126, 182–192. ( 10.1037/a0024555) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

19. Užgiris IC. 1981. Two functions of imitation during infancy. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 4, 1–12. ( 10.1177/016502548100400101) [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

20. Aboud FE. 2001. The formation of ingroup favoritism and outgroup prejudice in young children: are they distinct attitudes? Dev. Psychol. 39, 48–60. ( 10.1037/0012-1649.39.1.48) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

21. Bennett M, Sani F. 2008. Social identities in childhood: when does the group become a part of the self-concept? Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass 2, 1281–1296. ( 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2008.00105.x) [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

22. Nesdale D, Flesser D. 2001. Social identity and the development of children's group attitudes. Child Dev. 72, 506–517. ( 10.1111/1467-8624.00293) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

23. Turner JC, Hogg MA, Oakes PJ, Reicher SD, Wetherell MS. 1987. Rediscovering the social group: a self-categorization theory. New York, NY: Blackwell. [Google Scholar]

24. Klein O, Spears R, Reicher S. 2007. Social identity performance: extending the strategic side of the SIDE model. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 11, 28–45. ( 10.1177/1088868306294588) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

25. Master A, Walton GM. 2013. Minimal groups increase young children's motivation and learning on group-relevant tasks. Child Dev. 84, 737–751. ( 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01867.x) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

26. Baumeister RF, Leary MR. 1995. The need to belong: desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychol. Bull. 117, 497–529. ( 10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

27. Fiske ST. 2010. Social beings: core motives in social psychology, 2nd edn New York, NY: Wiley. [Google Scholar]

28. Knowles ML, Lucas GM, Molden DC, Gardner WL, Dean KK. 2010. There's no substitute for belonging: self-affirmation following social and non-social threats. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 36, 173–186. ( 10.1177/0146167209346860) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

29. Carr PB, Walton GM. 2014. Cues of working together fuel intrinsic motivation. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 53, 169–184. ( 10.1016/j.jesp.2014.03.015) [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

30. Walton GM. 2014. The new science of wise psychological interventions. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 23, 73–82. ( 10.1177/0963721413512856) [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

31. Williams KD. 2007. Ostracism. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 58, 425–452. ( 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085641) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

32. Dunham Y, Olson KR. 2008. The importance of origins: why cognitive development is central to a mature understanding of social psychology. Open Psychol. J. 1, 59–65. ( 10.2174/1874350100801010059) [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

33. Schacter S. 1959. The psychology of affiliation. Experimental studies of the sources of gregariousness. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. [Google Scholar]

34. Maslow AH. 1968. Toward a psychology of being. New York, NY: Van Nostrand. [Google Scholar]

35. Brewer MB. 1991. The social self: on being the same and different at the same time. Pers. Soc. Psychol. B 17, 475–482. ( 10.1177/0146167291175001) [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

36. Hagerty MK, Williams RA, Coyne JC, Early MR. 1996. Sense of belonging and indicators of social and psychological functioning. Arch. Psychiatr. Nurs. 10, 235–244. ( 10.1016/S0883-9417(96)80029-X) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

37. Mathes EW, Edwards LL. 1978. An empirical test of Maslow's theory of motivation. J. Humanist Psychol. 18, 75–77. ( 10.1177/002216787801800111) [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

38. Axelrod R. 1984. The evolution of cooperation. New York, NY: Basic Books. [Google Scholar]

39. Hamlin JK, Wynn K, Bloom P. 2007. Social evaluation by preverbal infants. Nature 450, 557–559. ( 10.1038/nature06288) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

40. Tomasello M, Carpenter M. 2007. Shared intentionality. Dev. Sci. 10, 121–125. ( 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00573.x) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

41. Bowlby J. 1973. Attachment and loss: vol. 2. Separation anxiety and anger. New York, NY: Basic Books. [Google Scholar]

42. White LO, Wu J, Borelli JL, Mayes LC, Crowley MJ. 2013. Play it again: neural responses to reunion with excluders predicted by attachment patterns. Dev. Sci. 16, 850–863. ( 10.1111/desc.12035) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

43. Anisfeld E. 1982. The onset of social smiling in preterm and full-term infants from two ethnic backgrounds. Infant Behav. Dev. 5, 387–395. ( 10.1016/S0163-6383(82)80048-9) [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

44. Emde RN, Harmon RJ. 1972. Endogenous and exogenous smiling systems in early infancy. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 11, 177–200. ( 10.1016/S0002-7138(10)60071-4) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

45. Gewirtz JL. 1965. The course of infant smiling in four child-rearing environments in Israel. In Determinants of infant behavior 111 (ed. Foss BM.), pp. 205–248. London, UK: Methuen. [Google Scholar]

46. Rochat P, Querido JG, Striano T. 1999. Emerging sensitivity to the timing and structure of protoconversation in early infancy. Dev. Psychol. 35, 950–957. ( 10.1037/0012-1649.35.4.950) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

47. Trevarthen C, Aitken KJ. 2001. Infant intersubjectivity: research, theory, and clinical applications. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 42, 3–48. ( 10.1111/1469-7610.00701) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

48. Carpenter M. 2010. Social cognition and social motivations in infancy. In The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development, 2nd edn (ed Goswami U.), pp. 106–128. Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell. [Google Scholar]

49. Over H, Carpenter M. 2013. The social side of imitation. Child Dev. Perspect. 7, 6–11. ( 10.1111/cdep.12006) [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

50. Lakin JL, Chartrand TL. 2003. Using nonconscious behavioral mimicry to create affiliation and rapport. Psychol. Sci. 14, 334–339. ( 10.1111/1467-9280.14481) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

51. Nielsen M. 2006. Copying actions and copying outcomes: social learning through the second year. Dev. Psychol. 42, 555–565. ( 10.1037/0012-1649.42.3.555) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

52. Nielsen M, Simcock G, Jenkins L. 2008. The effect of social engagement on 24-month-olds’ imitation from live and televised models. Dev. Sci. 11, 722–731. ( 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00722.x) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

53. Hay DF. 2009. The roots and branches of human altruism. Br. J. Psychol. 100, 473–479. ( 10.1348/000712609X442096) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

54. Martin A, Olson KR. 2015. Beyond good and evil: what motivations underlie children's prosocial behavior. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 10, 159–175. ( 10.1177/1745691615568998) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

56. Warneken F, Tomasello M. 2006. Altruistic helping in human infants and young chimpanzees. Science 311, 1301–1303. ( 10.1126/science.1121448) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

57. Warneken F, Tomasello M. 2007. Helping and cooperation at 14 months of age. Infancy 11, 271–294. ( 10.1080/15250000701310389) [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

58. Liszkowski U, Carpenter M, Tomasello M. 2008. Twelve-month-olds communicate helpfully and appropriately for knowledgeable and ignorant partners. Cognition 108, 732–739. ( 10.1016/j.cognition.2008.06.013) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

59. Warneken F, Tomasello M. 2008. Extrinsic rewards undermine altruistic tendencies in 20-month-olds. Dev. Psychol. 44, 1785–1788. ( 10.1037/a0013860) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

60. Callaghan T, Moll H, Rakoczy H, Warneken F, Liszkowski U, Behne T, Tomasello M. 2011. Early social cognition in three cultural contexts. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 76, vii–viii. ( 10.1111/j.1540-5834.2011.00603.x) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

61. Over H, Carpenter M. 2009. Eighteen-month-old infants show increased helping following priming with affiliation. Psychol. Sci. 20, 1189–1193. ( 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02419.x) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

62. Rekers Y, Haun DB, Tomasello M. 2011. Children, but not chimpanzees, prefer to collaborate. Curr. Biol. 21, 1756–1758. ( 10.1016/j.cub.2011.08.066) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

63. Butler LP, Walton GM. 2013. The opportunity to collaborate increases preschoolers’ motivation for challenging tasks. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 116, 953–961. ( 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.06.007) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

64. Engelmann JM, Herrmann E, Tomasello M. 2012. Five-year olds, but not chimpanzees, attempt to manage their reputations. PLoS ONE 7, 1–7. ( 10.1371/journal.pone.0048433) [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

65. Engelmann J, Over H, Herrmann E, Tomasello M. 2013. Young children care more about their reputation with ingroup members and possible reciprocators. Dev. Sci. 16, 552–558. ( 10.1111/desc.12086) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

66. Howes C, Matheson CC, Wu F. 1992. Friendships and social pretend play. In The collaborative construction of pretend (eds Howes C, Unger O, Matheson CC), pp. 107–115. Albany, NY: State University of New York Press. [Google Scholar]

67. Gifford-Smith M, Brownell C. 2003. Childhood peer relationships: social acceptance, friendships & peer networks. J. School Psychol. 41, 235–284. ( 10.1016/S0022-4405(03)00048-7) [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

68. Howes C. 1996. The earliest friendships. In The company they keep: friendship in childhood and adolescence (eds Bukowski WM, Newcomb AF, Hartup WW), pp. 66–86. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]

69. Newcomb AF, Bagwell CL. 1995. Children's friendship relations: a meta-analytic review. Psychol. Bull. 117, 306–347. ( 10.1037/0033-2909.117.2.306) [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

70. Newcomb AF, Bagwell CL. 1996. The developmental significance of children's friendship relations. In The company they keep: friendship in childhood and adolescence (eds Bukowski WM, Newcomb AF, Hartup WW), pp. 289–321. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]

71. Olson KR, Spelke ES. 2008. Foundations of cooperation in young children. Cognition 108, 222–231. ( 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.12.003) [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

72. Moore C. 2009. Fairness in resource allocation in young children depends on recipient. Psychol. Sci. 20, 944–948. ( 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02378.x) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

73. Hartup WW, Laursen B. 1999. Relationships as developmental contexts: retrospective themes and contemporary issues. In The Minnesota symposia on child psychology: vol. 29. Relationships as developmental contexts (eds Collins WA, Laursen B), pp. 13–35. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]

74. Hartup WW. 1996. The company they keep: friendships and their developmental significance. Child Dev. 67, 1–13. ( 10.2307/1131681) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

75. Verbeek P, de Waal FBM. 2001. Peacemaking among preschool children. J. Peace Psychol. 7, 5–28. ( 10.1207/S15327949PAC0701_02) [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

76. Smith CE, Harris PL. 2012. He didn't want me to feel sad: children's reactions to disappointment and apology. Soc. Dev. 21, 215–228. ( 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2011.0060) [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

77. Vaish A, Carpenter M, Tomasello M. 2011. Young children's responses to guilt displays. Dev. Psychol. 47, 1248–1262. ( 10.1037/a0024462) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

78. Talwar V, Murphy SM, Lee K. 2007. White lie-telling in children for politeness purposes. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 31, 1–11. ( 10.1177/0165025406073530) [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

79. Cole PM. 1986. Children's spontaneous control of facial expression. Child Dev. 57, 1309–1321. ( 10.2307/1130411) [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

80. Cole PM, Zahn-Waxler C, Smith KD. 1994. Expressive control during a disappointment: variations related to preschoolers’ behavior problems. Dev. Psychol. 30, 835–846. ( 10.1037/0012-1649.30.6.835) [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

81. Fu G, Lee K. 2007. Social grooming in kindergarten: the emergence of flattery behavior. Dev. Sci. 10, 255–265. ( 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00583.x) [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

82. Swann WB Jr, Gómez A, Seyle C, Morales F. 2009. Identity fusion: the interplay of personal and social identities in extreme group behavior. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 96, 995–1011. ( 10.1037/a0013668) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

83. Putallaz M, Wasserman A. 1989. Children's naturalistic entry behavior and sociometric status: a developmental perspective. Dev. Psychol. 25, 297–305. ( 10.1037/0012-1649.25.2.297) [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

84. Corriveau KH, Harris PL. 2010. Preschoolers (sometimes) defer to the majority in making simple perceptual judgments. Dev. Psychol. 46, 437–445. ( 10.1037/a0017553) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

85. Walker MB, Andrade MG. 1996. Conformity in the Asch task as a function of age. J. Soc. Psychol. 136, 367–372. ( 10.1080/00224545.1996.9714014) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

86. Haun DBM, Tomasello M. 2011. Conformity to peer pressure in preschool children. Child Dev. 82, 1759–1767. ( 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01666.x) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

87. Dunham Y, Baron A, Carey S. 2011. Consequences of ‘minimal’ group affiliations in children. Child Dev. 82, 793–811. ( 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01577.x) [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

88. Misch A, Over H, Carpenter M. 2015. Young children show loyalty to the group by keeping the group's secrets. J. Exp. Child Psychol. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

89. Killen M, Rutland A. 2011. Children and social exclusion: morality, prejudice and group identity. Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell. [Google Scholar]

90. House JS, Landis K, Umberson D. 1988. Social relationships and health. Science 241, 540–545. ( 10.1126/science.3399889) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

91. Williams KD, Cheung CKT, Choi W. 2000. Cyberostracism: effects of being ignored over the Internet. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 79, 748–762. ( 10.1037/0022-3514.79.5.748) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

92. Wesselmann ED, Cardoso F, Slater S, Williams KD. 2012. ‘To be looked at as though air’: civil attention matters. Psychol. Sci. 23, 166–168. ( 10.1177/0956797611427921) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

93. Wirth J, Sacco DF, Hugenberg K, Williams KD. 2010. Eye gaze as relational evaluation: averted eye gaze leads to feelings of ostracism and relational devaluation. Pers. Soc. Psychol. B 36, 869–882. ( 10.1177/0146167210370032) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

94. Tronick EZ. 2007. The neurobehavioral and social-emotional development of infants and children. New York, NY: Norton. [Google Scholar]

95. Abrams D, Weick M, Thomas D, Colbe H, Franklin KM. 2011. Online ostracism affects children differently from adolescents and adults. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 29, 110–123. ( 10.1348/026151010X494089) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

96. Crowley MJ, Wu J, Molfese PJ, Mayes LC. 2010. Social exclusion in middle childhood: rejection events, slow-wave neural activity, and ostracism distress. Soc. Neurosci. 5, 483–495. ( 10.1080/17470919.2010.500169) [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

97. Nesdale D, Lambert A. 2007. Effects of experimentally manipulated peer rejection on children's negative affect, self-esteem, and maladaptive social behavior. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 31, 115–122. ( 10.1177/0165025407073579) [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

98. Cullerton-Sen C, Crick NR. 2005. Understanding the effects of physical and relational victimization: the utility of multiple perspectives in predicting social emotional adjustment. School Psychol. Rev. 34, 147–160. [Google Scholar]

99. Over H, Carpenter M. 2009. Priming third-party ostracism increases affiliative imitation in children. Dev. Sci. 12, F1–F8. ( 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00820.x) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

100. Warneken F, Chen F, Tomasello M. 2006. Cooperative activities in young children and chimpanzees. Child Dev. 77, 640–663. ( 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00895.x) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

101. Ross HS, Lollis SP. 1987. Communication within infant social games. Dev. Psychol. 23, 241–248. ( 10.1037/0012-1649.23.2.241) [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

102. Watson-Jones RE, Legare CH, Whitehouse H, Clegg JM. 2014. Task-specific effects of ostracism on imitation in early childhood. Evol. Hum. Behav. 35, 204–210. ( 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2014.01.004) [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

103. Song R, Over H, Carpenter M. 2015. Children draw more affiliative pictures following priming with ostracism. Dev. Psychol. 51, 831–840. ( 10.1037/a0039176) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

104. Dawson G, Toth K, Robert A, Osterling J, Munson J, Estes A, Liaw J. 2004. Early social attention impairments in autism: social orienting, joint attention, and attention to distress. Dev. Psychol. 40, 271–283. ( 10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.271) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

105. Dubey I, Ropar D, Hamilton AFC. 2015. Measuring the value of social engagement in adults with and without autism. Mol. Autism 6, 1–9. ( 10.1186/s13229-015-0031-2) [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

106. Baron-Cohen S. 1995. Mindblindness. Boston, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]

107. Frith U. 1989. Autism and ‘theory of mind‘. In Diagnosis and treatment of autism (ed. Gillberg C.), pp. 33–52. New York, NY: Plenum Press. [Google Scholar]

108. Liebal K, Colombi C, Rogers SJ, Warneken F, Tomasello M. 2008. Helping and cooperation in children with autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 38, 224–238. ( 10.1007/s10803-007-0381-5) [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

109. Swinkels SH, Dietz C, van Daalen E, Kerkhof IH, van Engeland H, Buitelaar JK. 2006. Screening for autistic spectrum disorder in children aged 14 to 15 months. I: the development of the early screening of autistic traits questionnaire (ESAT). J. Autism Dev. Disord. 36, 723–732. ( 10.1007/s10803-006-0115-0) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

110. Leekam SR, Ramsden CAH. 2006. Dyadic orienting and joint attention in preschool children with autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 36, 185–197. ( 10.1007/s10803-005-0054-1) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

111. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S. 2003. The friendship questionnaire: an investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism and normal sex differences. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 33, 509–517. ( 10.1023/A:1025879411971) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

112. Chevallier C, Parish-Morris J, Tonge N, Le L, Miller J, Schultz RT. 2014. Susceptibility to the audience effect explains performance gap between children with and without autism in a theory of mind task. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 143, 972–979. ( 10.1037/a0035483) [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

113. Walton GM, Cohen GL. 2011. A brief social-belonging intervention improves academic and health outcomes of minority students. Science 331, 1447–1451. ( 10.1126/science.1198364) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

114. Cook JE, Purdie-Vaughns V, Garcia J, Cohen GL. 2012. Chronic threat and contingent belonging: protective benefits of values affirmation on identity development. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 102, 479–496. ( 10.1037/a0026312) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

115. Plötner M, Over H, Carpenter M, Tomasello M. 2015. Young children show the bystander effect in helping situations. Psychol. Sci. 26, 499–506. ( 10.1177/0956797615569579) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

116. Vaish A, Carpenter M, Tomasello M. 2009. Sympathy through affective perspective-taking and its relation to prosocial behavior in toddlers. Dev. Psychol. 45, 534–543. ( 10.1037/a0014322) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

117. Martin J, Bennett M, Murray WS. 2008. A developmental study of the infrahumanization hypothesis. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 26, 153–161. ( 10.1348/026151007X216261) [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

118. Hilbrink E, Sakkalou E, Ellis-Davies K, Fowler N, Gattis ML. 2013. Selective and faithful imitation at 12 and 15 months. Dev. Sci. 16, 828–840. ( 10.1111/desc.12070) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

119. Benenson JF, Pascoe J, Radmore N. 2007. Children's altruistic behavior in the dictator game. Evol. Hum. Behav. 28, 168–175. ( 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2006.10.003) [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

120. Eisenberg N, Fabes RA. 1998. Prosocial development. In Handbook of child psychology: social, emotional and personality development, 5th edn, vol. 4 (eds Eisenberg N, Damon W), pp. 701–778. New York, NY: Wiley. [Google Scholar]

121. Manktelow KI. 2012. Thinking and reasoning: psychological perspectives on reason, judgment, and decision making. Hove, UK: Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]

122. Steinbeis N, Bernhardt BC, Singer T. 2012. Impulse control and underlying functions of the left DLPFC mediate age-related and age-independent individual differences in strategic social behavior. Neuron 73, 1040–1051. ( 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.12.027) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

123. Over H, Over DE. In press. Deontic reasoning and social norms: broader implications. In The thinking mind: The uses of thinking in everyday life (eds Galbraith N, Lucas E, Over DE). Hove, UK: Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]

124. Pfundmair M, Aydin N, Du H, Yeung S, Frey D, Graupmann V. 2015. Exclude me if you can – cultural effects on the outcomes of social exclusion. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 46, 579–596. ( 10.1177/0022022115571203) [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

125. Greenfield P, Keller H, Fuligni A, Maynard A. 2003. Cultural pathways through universal development. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 54, 461–490. ( 10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145221) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

126. Over H, Uskul AK. Submitted. Culture influences children's responses to ostracism situations. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

127. Iyengar SS, Lepper MR. 1999. Rethinking the value of choice: a cultural perspective on intrinsic motivation. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 76, 349–366. ( 10.1037/0022-3514.76.3.349) [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Which type of friendships is activity centered?

Agentic friendships, on the other hand, were activity-centered.

Which of the following is the main situational factor that affects friendship formation?

The main situational factor that may facilitate or impede friendship formation is availability.

Which is a hallmark of a successful friendship?

Loyalty, trust, equality are hallmarks of strong friendship.

Which friendship style is based on a deep connection regardless of changing life circumstances?

The second type of friendship discussed by Matthews was the discerning style, which, unlike independents, is marked by a deep connection with a friend or group of friends regardless of changing circumstances in their lives.

Toplist

Neuester Beitrag

Stichworte