What can you reliably predict about normal age related changes in cognitive functioning in middle adulthood?

Abstract

Large population-based studies on the associations of childhood factors with late-life cognition are lacking in many low and middle income countries including India. In this study, we assessed the prevalence of late-life cognitive impairment and examined the associations of childhood socioeconomic status (SES) and health conditions with cognitive impairment among older adults in India. Data for this study were derived from the Longitudinal Ageing Study in India conducted in 2017–18. The effective sample size was 31,464 older adults aged 60 years and above. Cognitive functioning was measured through five global domains (memory, orientation, arithmetic function, executive function, and object naming). The overall score ranged between 0 and 43, and the score was reversed indicating cognitive impairment. Descriptive statistics along with mean scores of cognitive impairment were presented. Additionally, moderated multivariable linear regression models were employed to examine the association between explanatory variables, including childhood SES and health conditions and late-life cognitive impairment. The mean score of cognitive functioning among the study participants was 21.72 (CI 2.64–21.80). About 15% of older adults had poor health conditions, and 44% had lower financial status during their childhood. Older adults who had a fair health during their childhood were more likely to suffer from cognitive impairment in comparison to older adults who had good health during their childhood (Coef: 0.60; CI 0.39, 0.81). In comparison to older adults who had good childhood financial status, those who had poor childhood financial status were more likely to suffer from cognitive impairment (Coef: 0.81; CI 0.56, 1.07). Older adults who had fair childhood health status and poor childhood financial status were more likely to suffer from cognitive impairment in comparison to older adults who had good childhood health and good financial status (Coef: 1.26; CI 0.86, 1.66). Social policies such as improving educational and financial resources in disadvantaged communities and socioeconomically poor children and their families, would help to enhance a better cognitive ageing and a healthy and dignified life in old age.

Introduction

The growing number of older adults around the world poses serious challenges related to health, and older adults without any social security benefits, savings, and affordability of good quality care are the ones to suffer the most1. The risk of cognitive impairment is expected to be the most prominent with increasing age, and to a great extent, determined by childhood cognitive function2,3,4. However, studies differ substantially in identifying the factors responsible for faster cognitive decline with age5. Studies commonly identified childhood socioeconomic status as a representative of childhood adversities, while the association they found were inconsistent; for instance, a study found higher childhood socioeconomic status showing with the slower global cognitive decline with increasing age, whereas others found a faster cognitive decline6,7.

In recent years, the concentration of cognitive impairment cases such as dementia among older adults is rising8,9,10. However, the burden differs between subgroups in communities across countries based on abilities that can be categorized into memory, conceptualization, attention, language, knowledge, and spatial ability, each one of which is influenced by both biological as well as environmental factors11. Although the mechanisms have not been explicated, investigators have hypothesized that chronic diseases, functional disabilities, poor health behaviours and decreased social interactions may be potential mediators of childhood deprivations resulting in poor mental health in late-life12,13,14. Several longitudinal and interventional studies have reported the beneficial effects of non-pharmacological interventions such as resistance, aerobic and balance exercises on cognitive functions of older adults15,16. Furthermore, cognitive decline is also associated with numerous other factors such as age, gender, marital status, level of education, and overall health conditions in the past17. Some studies explained how marital status and a higher level of education motivated individuals to maintain a healthier lifestyle; that stimulates brain activities in day-to-day lives, and community participation/social involvement during the absence of a partner/spouse might be associated with better mental wellbeing18,19,20.

India is home to millions of children who are exposed to deprivation in terms of basic health, nutrition that facilitate survival, growth and development21. The poor state of children in India can be reflected by the increase in the cases of stunting, wasting and malnutrition in recent years22. Further, children who were being more exposed to early life deprivation were also associated with the development of lower cognitive reserve for the brain that might lead to poor learning experience and higher functional illiteracy23. Childhood with socio-economic disadvantage might also lead to pathways towards clinical and social risks such as substance use, social isolation and mental distress, which might worsen late-life cognitive function24. According to brain reserve hypothesis, environment plays an important role that influences brain plasticity and thus affects the intelligence, education and occupation that determine the cognitive functioning25,26. However, some studies argued that cognitive decline in older age is not determined by single life-period, rather due to exposure to reserve-related factors27, and the accumulation of multiple traumas throughout the life course28.

Since early life factors for late-onset diseases have been well documented in the epidemiological field29,30, understanding the independent association of early life factors with late-life cognition may be more beneficial in designing cognition-enhancing interventions which may be equally effective regardless of individuals’ genetic susceptibility27. Although it has received empirical attention in several developed countries31,32,33, and some developing countries like China11,34,35, South Africa36, and Indonesia37, large population-based studies on the associations of childhood factors with late-life cognition are lacking in many low and middle income countries including India. In this study, we tested whether childhood deprivation is associated with cognitive impairment in older age, using data from the survey on older adults with comprehensive information on their socioeconomic and health status. In the analysis, we focused on markers of childhood deprivation, including childhood health conditions and early life family financial status of older Indian adults.

Methods

Data

Data for this study were derived from the recent release of the Longitudinal Ageing Study in India (LASI) wave 1. The LASI is a full-scale national survey of scientific investigation of the health, economic, and social determinants and consequences of population ageing in India, conducted in 2017–18 by the International Institute for Population Sciences (IIPS) in partnership with national and international institutions38. The LASI is a nationally representative survey of 72,250 individuals aged 45 and above across all states and union territories of India. The main objective of the survey is to study the health status and the social and economic well-being of older adults in India. LASI adopted a multistage stratified area probability cluster sampling design to arrive at the eventual units of observation: older adults age 45 and above and their spouses irrespective of age. The survey adopted a three-stage sampling design in rural areas and a four-stage sampling design in urban areas. In each state/Union Territory, the first stage involved the selection of Primary Sampling Units (PSUs), that is, sub-districts (Tehsils/Talukas), and the second stage involved the selection of villages in rural areas and wards in urban areas in the selected PSUs. In rural areas, households were selected from selected villages in the third stage. However, sampling in urban areas involved an additional stage. Specifically, in the third stage, one Census Enumeration Block (CEB) was randomly selected in each urban area. In the fourth stage, households were selected from this CEB. The socio-demographic and health-related information of respondents in the LASI survey was assessed using the face-to-face interviews which were conducted using computer-assisted personal interview (CAPI). The detailed methodology, with the complete information on the survey design and data collection, was published in the survey report38. The present study is conducted on eligible respondents aged 60 years and above. The total sample size for the present study is 31,464 (15,098 males and 16,366 females) elderly persons aged 60 years and above. Figure 1 represents the flowchart for the study sample selection. All methods were performed in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations.

Figure 1

What can you reliably predict about normal age related changes in cognitive functioning in middle adulthood?

Flowchart for the study sample selection.

Full size image

Variable description

Outcome variable

Cognitive functioning was assessed using continuous measures of five global domains of cognition (memory, orientation, arithmetic function, executive function, and object naming), adapted from the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE)39, and the cognitive module of the Health and Retirement Study, the China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study (CHARLS), and the Mexican Health and Aging Study (MHAS)40,41. Memory was measured using immediate word recall and delayed word recall. Orientation was measured using time and place measures. The arithmetic function was measured through backward counting, a serial seven subtraction task and a task involving two computations38,40. Additionally, paper folding (folding a piece of paper according to instructions), pentagon drawing (drawing intersecting circles) and object naming methods were followed to measure the cognitive functions among older adults41 (Cronbach’s alpha: 0.70). The overall score of composite index of cognitive function ranged between 0 and 43, and a higher score indicated better cognitive functioning. Further, for the analytical purpose, the score was reversed to assess the cognitive impairment among older adults and thus after reversing, the higher score indicated higher levels of cognitive impairment. In our study, the respondents who received assistance during the cognition module were excluded from the analysis38.

Key explanatory variables

The main explanatory variables were childhood health status (good, fair and poor) and childhood SES (good, average, poor). In the LASI survey, childhood health condition was measured using the question “Now I want to ask you about your overall childhood health up to age 16. In general, would you say your childhood health was very good, good, fair, poor, or very poor based on what you remember, or what you heard or perceived from your parents?” The responses were coded as good if he/she reported “very good and good” and fair if he/she reported “fair” and poor if he/she reported “poor and very poor”42. Additionally, financial status of the respondents’ family during childhood (childhood SES) was measured through the question, “Now, think about your family when you were growing up, from birth to age 16. Compared to other families in your community, would you say your family during that time was pretty well off financially, about average, or poor?” The response was coded as good if they reported “pretty well off financially”, average if reported “average” and poor if reported “poor”.

Mediating factors

As per the above-mentioned literature, the following factors which were shown to potentially mediate the associations between childhood factors and late-life cognition were included in the current analysis. Social participation was measured through the question- “Are you a member of any of the organizations, religious groups, clubs, or societies?” and the response was coded as no and yes. Physical activity was categorized as frequent (every day), rare (more than once a week, once a week, one to three times in a month), and never. The question through which physical activity was assessed was “How often do you take part in sports or vigorous activities, such as running or jogging, swimming, going to a health centre or gym, cycling, or digging with a spade or shovel, heavy lifting, chopping, farm work, fast bicycling, cycling with loads?”43. If the older adult was ill-treated in the last 1 year, then it was coded as “yes”; otherwise “no”.

The probable major depression among older adults with symptoms of dysphoria, was calculated using the CIDI-SF (Short Form Composite International Diagnostic Interview) with a score of 3 or more indicating “diagnosed with depression”44. The Cronbach’s alpha value for the CIDI-SF scale was 0.668. This scale estimates a probable psychiatric diagnosis of major depression and has been validated in field settings and widely used in population-based health surveys38. Self-rated health was coded as good which includes excellent, very good, and good whereas poor includes fair and poor45. Difficulty in activities of daily living (ADL) was coded as no and yes. The Cronbach’s alpha value for ADL scale was 0.869. ADL is a term used to refer to normal daily self-care activities (such as movement in bed, changing position from sitting to standing, feeding, bathing, dressing, grooming, personal hygiene, etc.). Difficulty in instrumental ADL (IADL) was coded as no and yes46. The Cronbach’s alpha value for IADL scale was 0.879. These include activities that are not necessarily related to the fundamental functioning of a person, but they let an individual live independently in a community. Morbidity was coded as no morbidity, 1 and 2 + 47. The variable morbidity was created using the data on chronic diseases which include hypertension, chronic heart diseases, stroke, any chronic lung disease, diabetes, cancer or malignant tumor, any bone/joint disease, neurological/psychiatric disease, or high cholesterol47.

Control variables

Several socio-demographic variables were controlled in the analysis. They include, age, which was recoded as young old (60–69 years), old-old (70–79 years), and oldest-old (80 + years); sex, which was recoded as male and female; educational status (equivalent to the International Standard Classification of Education (ISCED) categories)48, which was recoded as no education/primary not completed, primary (ISCED 1), secondary (ISCED 2, 3 and 4) and higher; working status, which was recoded as never worked, currently working, currently not working and retired; marital status, which was categorized as currently married, widowed, and others (divorced/separated/never married); and living arrangement, which was categorized as living alone, living with a spouse, living with spouse and children, and living with others.

Further, the monthly per capita consumption expenditure (MPCE) quintile was assessed using household consumption data. Sets of 11 and 29 questions on the expenditures on food and non-food items, respectively, were used to canvas the sample households. Food expenditure was collected based on a reference period of seven days, and non-food expenditure was collected based on reference periods of 30 days and 365 days. Food and non-food expenditures have been standardized to the 30-day reference period. The MPCE is computed and used as the summary measure of consumption38. The variable was divided into five quintiles i.e., from poorest to richest. Religion was coded as Hindu, Muslim, Christian, and Others. Caste was recoded as Scheduled Tribe (ST), Scheduled Caste (SC), Other Backward Classes (OBCs), and others. The ST refers to a large number of aboriginal ethnic groups or the indigenous population in the country. The SC includes the population that is socially segregated and financially/economically weak by their low status as per the Hindu caste hierarchy. The STs and SCs are among the most disadvantaged and discriminated socio-economic groups as per Government of India official classification. The OBC is the group of people who were identified as “educationally, economically and socially backwards”. The OBCs are considered low in the traditional caste hierarchy but are higher in status than the STs/SCs. The “other” caste category is identified as those who are having higher social status, mostly belonging to the upper caste Hindus49. The place of residence was coded as rural and urban. The regions of India were coded as North, Central, East, Northeast, West, and South.

Statistical analysis

Descriptive statistics along with mean (95% confidence interval) was presented in the study. Additionally, moderated multiple linear regression analysis50 was used to examine the association between the outcome variable (cognitive impairment) with other explanatory variables. The estimates were presented in the form of adjusted coefficients calculated at 95% confidence interval (CI). Additionally, standard beta coefficients were presented in the results. The regression diagnostics for heteroscedasticity51, multicollinearity52, and outliers were performed via computation of variance inflation factors (VIFs) and visual inspection of residual plots for the regression models. The complex survey design effects were adjusted by using STATA svyset and svy commands. The whole statistical analyses were performed by using Stata version 1453. Model-1 provides the estimates adjusted for all the mediating and control variables considered in the study. Model-2 represents the adjusted estimates of interaction effects of childhood health (good fair, and poor) and childhood financial status (good, average, poor) with cognitive impairment among older adults. Model-3 represents the estimates from the stratified analysis (categorical results) of childhood health and childhood financial status.

The Indian Council of Medical Research (ICMR), Delhi and Institutional Review boards (IRBs) of all partner institutions extended the necessary guidance and ethical approval for conducting the LASI survey. The partner institutions included IIPS, Mumbai; Harvard T.H. Chan School of Public Health (HSPH), Boston; University of Southern California (USC), Los Angeles; ICMR-National AIDS Research Institute (NARI), Pune; and the Regional Geriatric Centres (RGCs), ministry of health and family welfare (MoHFW). Informed consent was obtained from all subjects and/or their legal guardian(s) in accordance with human subject protection protocols.

Results

Table 1 represents the socioeconomic profile of the older adults in India. It was found that about 3.7% of the older adults had poor health conditions in their childhood. Nearly 43.9% of the older adults had a poor childhood SES. Around 68.0% of the older adults had no education or their primary education was incomplete. Nearly 26.4% of the older adults were currently not working. About 36.2% of older adults were widowed, whereas about 61.6% were currently married. Nearly 5.7% of older adults lived alone, and about 20.3% lived with their spouses. About 95.5% of older adults had no social participation. Nearly 69.3% of the older adults reported that they have never done any physical activity. Almost 5.2% of the older adults were ill-treated in last 1 year. About 8.7% of the older adults were suffering from depression. Nearly 48.6% of the older adults reported having poor self-rated health. About 23.8% and 48.3% of the older adults reported having difficulty in ADL and IADL, respectively. About 23.9% of the older adults had 2 + chronic conditions.

Table 1 Socio-economic profile of older adults in India, (n = 31,464).

Full size table

Table 2 represents the mean score of cognitive impairment by background characteristics. It was found that the mean score of cognitive impairment was higher among older adults whose childhood status was fair (mean: 22.71; CI 22.46, 22.96). The mean score of cognitive impairment was higher among older adults who had poor financial status of family during their childhood (mean: 23.29; CI 23.16, 23.41). The mean score of cognitive impairment was higher among older adults who had no education (mean: 24.56; CI 24.47, 24.65). Older adults who never worked had higher mean score of cognitive impairment (mean: 22.91; CI 22.75, 23.10). The mean score of cognitive impairment was reported to be higher among older adults who were widowed (mean: 23.88; CI 23.73, 24.02). Older adults who were living with others had higher mean score of cognitive impairment (mean: 24.30; CI 23.93, 24.67). The mean score of cognitive impairment was higher among older adults who had no social participation (mean: 21.88; CI 21.8, 21.97). Older adults who never did physical activity had higher mean score of cognitive impairment (mean: 22.21; CI 22.11, 22.31). The mean score of cognitive impairment was higher among older adults who were ill-treated in last one year (mean: 22.48; CI 22.06, 22.89). Older adults who had depression had a higher mean score of cognitive impairment (mean: 22.85; CI 22.53, 23.16). The mean score of cognitive impairment was higher among older adults who reported to have poor self-rated health (mean: 22.66; CI 22.54, 22.78) and those who had difficulty in ADL and IADL (mean: 23.73; CI 23.54, 23.93) (mean: 23.70; CI 23.57, 23.82).

Table 2 Mean score of cognitive impairment among older adults by background characteristics, (n = 24,625).

Full size table

Table 3 depicts the regression estimates of cognitive impairment among older adults by their background characteristics. Older adults who had a fair health during their childhood were more likely to suffer from cognitive impairment in reference to older adults who had good health during their childhood (Coef: 0.60; CI 0.39, 0.81). In comparison to older adults who had good childhood financial status, those who had poor childhood financial status were more likely to suffer from cognitive impairment (Coef: 0.81; CI 0.56, 1.07). The likelihood of cognitive impairment was found to be higher among the older adults who had no education in reference to older adults whose education status was higher (Coef: 6.99; CI 6.71, 7.26). Older adults who were widowed were more likely to suffer from cognitive impairment as compared to their married counterparts (Coef: 0.73; CI 0.56, 0.91). Older adults who were currently working were more likely to suffer from cognitive impairment as compared to older adults who never worked (Coef: 0.43; CI 0.22, 0.64). The likelihood of cognitive impairment was higher among older adults who had no social participation in comparison to those who had social participation (Coef: 0.69; CI 0.43, 0.94). Older adults who never involved in physical activity were more likely to suffer from cognitive impairment in reference to older adults who frequently involved in physical activity (Coef: 0.91; CI 0.73, 1.09). The likelihood of cognitive impairment was higher among older adults who had poor self-rated health in comparison to those who had good self-rated health (Coef: 0.72; CI 0.59, 0.86). Older adults who had difficulty in ADL and IADL were more likely to suffer from cognitive impairment in reference to older adults who had no difficulty in ADL and IADL (Coef: 0.66; CI 0.48, 0.84) (Coef: 0.98; CI 0.83, 1.12).

Table 3 Regression estimates of cognitive impairment among older adults by their background characteristics, (n = 24,625).

Full size table

Model-2 represents the interaction estimates of childhood health and financial status on cognitive impairment among older adults. Although the actual main effects were significant, there was no statistical significance in the interaction effects. Further, Model-3 represents the stratified analysis of childhood health and childhood financial status. Older adults who had fair childhood health and poor childhood financial status were more likely to suffer from cognitive impairment in comparison to older adults who had good childhood health and good financial status (Coef: 1.26; CI 0.86, 1.66). Table S1 (supplementary file) presents the regression estimates for cognitive impairment among older adults stratified by sex along with the results of interaction and stratification of childhood factors with late-life cognitive impairment; additionally, Table S2 (supplementary file) represents the sensitivity analysis for mild cognitive impairment among older adults after excluding participants with suspected dementia by their background characteristics. Similar results were observed in this additional analysis.

Discussion

It is fairly well established that early life childhood deprivation, environment and childhood health may contribute to cognitive impairment in the later life stages54. However, the risk of cognitive impairment in old age due to adverse socioeconomic conditions in childhood has been understudied in India compared to other developing countries. Such a study is particularly important in a resource-constrained setting where there is a need for wider efforts to reduce the prevalence of cognitive impairment among older adults and its burden on health care systems. This study used data from a large population-based ageing survey conducted in India, to advance understanding of childhood socioeconomic and health conditions as major factors in the early-life course that associate with cognitive function in later years of life.

As evidence suggests, children from households with higher SES may be in a more cognitively stimulating environment in their early life resulting in more advanced brain development than their disadvantaged counterparts55. Such advancements in the brain in the early life course are shown to be associated with better cognitive functioning in older ages56. Studies drawing on data from different socio-cultural settings had found that older adults, when they experienced higher levels of SES in childhood, perform better on neurocognitive tests57. On the other hand, it is documented that the longer people live in poor SES and health conditions, the greater would be their academic deficits and the more severe the decline in their cognitive abilities58. Concordantly, the present analysis provides consistent evidence that childhood SES predicts cognitive impairment in older ages. The experiences in childhood do substantially influence the health status in later life because childhood conditions predict to a great extent, the probable pathways that may lead to good or bad health. As multiple studies suggest, childhood economic resources and health determine the living and working conditions in adulthood, and those circumstances give rise to social inequalities in health59,60. Besides, nutritional deprivation during such important periods of early development may have negative effects on cognitive functioning in the long term34. Parallel to these findings, current results also suggest that compared to good childhood health, fair health condition is significantly associated with cognitive impairment among older adults. Both findings could be interpreted as evidence that childhood SES and health conditions may have a long-lasting effect on an active cognitive reserve that may have a major role in determining the rate of cognitive functioning in later years of life.

In the interaction analysis model that included a term for the interaction of childhood health and financial status, we did not find evidence for both the childhood adversities in combination increasing the significance effect on cognitive impairment. However, additional regression analysis including stratification of low childhood SES measured by a worse-off family financial status and fair childhood health conditions showed that they were statistically significant for cognitive impairment in old age. All these suggest that the effects of low childhood SES on late-life cognitive impairment were stronger for people with fair childhood health conditions than for people with good childhood health. Although fair childhood health status in the stratified estimates showed higher cognitive impairment, poor childhood health status did not show statistical significance which might be attributed to the lower sample size in the poor health category that might result in lack of statistical power in the analysis. Further longitudinal studies are warranted in developing countries like India that explore the rate of cognitive decline in old age in relation to the life course socioeconomic and health conditions. Such an investigation may further contribute to an improved understanding of the mechanisms such as lack of social and economic resources and increased illiteracy surrounding the cognitive impairment in old age and bringing interventions for early detection and prevention of cognitive impairment and related disabilities in older ages. The study also supports that the association of early life circumstances with cognitive characteristics in old age observed in high-income countries and some developing countries, including China, may extend to community-dwelling older adults in the Indian context as well.

Another particularly striking finding in our analysis was the protective effect of education on a late-life cognitive impairment that is consistent with past literature61,62,63, indicating that providing education as an intervention to diminish the adverse effects of poor childhood SES and health conditions on cognitive ageing. Significantly increased odds of lower education in relationship with cognitive impairment in our analysis support the findings of previous studies suggesting that the higher levels of education often lead to occupations that necessitate active cognitive involvements, which could further enhance or maintain cognitive functioning in late adulthood62,64. At the same time, children may lack the energy and motor skills essential to thrive in school due to poor household conditions and limited resources and thus complete fewer years of schooling, which in turn affects late-life cognition65,66. Hence, considering the findings of the present study, adverse household conditions could be an indicator for identifying the children at-risk who would benefit in the long term from targeted interventions on increasing their education.

The current findings revealed a significant female disadvantage in cognition and a stronger association of childhood health conditions with late-life cognitive functioning among women than men, where older women with a fair health status in childhood had higher odds of cognitive impairment in late-life compared to those with a good health status during childhood. This is consistent with previous studies67 which suggest that women are at higher disadvantage in terms of having adverse structural, behavioural and psychosocial characteristics across the lifespan that are related to poor late-life health outcomes. Again, the current finding is similar (for childhood SES) to existing studies which showed that childhood SES was associated with old-age mental health among women but not among men in models fully adjusted with adulthood SES and risk factors68,69. Considering the interaction results segregated by gender, having a fair childhood health and poor childhood SES had higher odds of cognitive impairment both among men and women in comparison to having a good health and SES in childhood and the odds were greater among women. On the other hand, a study based on the data from the HRS suggested that with respect to memory function, cumulative SES from childhood to adulthood may be more important among men than women70, which suggests the need for future studies on the influence of cumulative exposure to life-course disadvantages on late-life health with special focus on gender aspect.

There are several limitations of the present study to be acknowledged. Foremost, the cross-sectional design of the analysis in the present study prevents bringing out any causal inferences. Further, although a global measure of cognitive impairment has the advantage of assessing overall cognition, the relationship with early life health and SES is potentially different for specific domains of cognitive function. For example, memory unlike other cognitive functions is found to be more sensitive measure of age-related cognitive deficits71,72. Therefore, future studies on domain-specific associations are warranted. A sensitivity analysis was conducted in the current study after excluding participants who were cognitively impaired or suspected with dementias, and the results showed no changes in the observed associations. Also, measurement error in several cognitive domains may be biasing the current results due to a higher proportion of illiterate population in India (with 68% older adults with no or uncompleted primary education in this study), and thus additional research with longitudinal and interventional designs is required to unravel this issue. Notably, educational variable and its categories in the current study are equivalent to the ISCED and allow comparisons to be made with other international studies. Similarly, studies on the validity and reliability of the measurement method of cognitive impairment are recommended.

Indeed, it is also important to consider that some childhood conditions may affect cognitive functioning directly, and others may act indirectly through several pathways in adulthood6,73. Hence, understanding SES in adulthood as a risk/protective factor for later-life cognition is also essential for identifying the factors related to cognitive ability in older ages. Also, self-report of health conditions and SES in childhood may be subject to recall bias and information on receiving healthcare support/assistance for reported poor health conditions was not available which may bias the current results. This study, however, provides baseline data for understanding the ageing trajectories and the risk factors for cognitive impairment in late life. Further longitudinal studies with more follow-up information from upcoming waves of LASI surveys may add to this gap. Another major limitation is that given the predictor variables of interest in the study are self-reported, there are greater chances of recall bias, especially in the case of childhood conditions. However, the study has the credit of utilizing the large survey information of the older population, which is nationally representative and provides comprehensive measures of cognitive functioning in an ageing population.

Conclusions

The current study's findings highlight the necessity of determining whether certain developmental periods are linked to cognitive impairment later in life. Our findings also imply that governments should place a greater emphasis on closing socioeconomic resource inequalities across the lifespan, especially in childhood. Furthermore, there are various windows of opportunity for age-based interventions, with those in the early years of life shaping individuals' socioeconomic paths into later life being the most promising. As a result, social measures such as increasing educational and financial resources in disadvantaged neighbourhoods and socioeconomically poor children and their families may aid in cognitive ageing and a healthy and dignified life in old age. Without a question, socioeconomic measures aimed at improving childhood conditions are critical, as here is where an incremental route to long-term physical as well as mental health begins.

Data availability

The datasets generated and/or analysed during the current study are available in the International Institute for Population Institute’s repository, [https://iipsindia.ac.in/sites/default/files/LASI_DataRequestForm_0.pdf].

Abbreviations

IADL:

Instrumental activities of daily living

ADL:

Activities of daily living

MPCE:

Monthly per capita consumption expenditure

SES:

Socio-economic status

LASI:

Longitudinal Ageing Study in India

CIDI-SF:

Short Form Composite International Diagnostic Interview

References

  1. WHO. World Report on Ageing and Health (2017).

  2. Gale, C. R. et al. Cognitive function in childhood and lifetime cognitive change in relation to mental wellbeing in four cohorts of older people. PLoS ONE 7, 1–9 (2012).

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Murman DL. The impact of age on cognition. In Seminars in hearing. Thieme Medical Publishers. 36(03), 111–121 (2015).

    Google Scholar 

  4. Liu, Y. & Lachman, M. E. Socioeconomic status and parenting style from childhood: long-term effects on cognitive function in middle and later adulthood. J. Gerontol. Ser. B 74, e13–e24 (2019).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Blondell, S. J., Hammersley-Mather, R. & Veerman, J. L. Does physical activity prevent cognitive decline and dementia? A systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. BMC Public Health 14, 1–12 (2014).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Lyu, J. & Burr, J. A. Socioeconomic status across the life course and cognitive function among older adults: An examination of the latency, pathways, and accumulation hypotheses. J. Aging Health 28, 40–67 (2016).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Aartsen, M. J. et al. Advantaged socioeconomic conditions in childhood are associated with higher cognitive functioning but stronger cognitive decline in older age. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1807679116 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Hampel, H. & Lista, S. Dementia: The rising global tide of cognitive impairment. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 12, 131–132 (2016).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Livingston, G. A, Sommerlad V, Orgeta SG, et al. The Lancet International Commission on Dementia Prevention and Care. Lancet; 390.

  10. Mukadam, N. et al. Population attributable fractions for risk factors for dementia in low-income and middle-income countries: An analysis using cross-sectional survey data. Lancet Glob. Health https://doi.org/10.1016/S2214-109X(19)30074-9 (2019).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Sha, T., Yan, Y. & Cheng, W. Associations of childhood socioeconomic status with mid-life and late-life cognition in Chinese middle-aged and older population based on a 5-year period cohort study. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 33, 1335–1345 (2018).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Crimmins, E. M., Kim, J. K. & Seeman, T. E. Poverty and biological risk: The earlier ‘aging’ of the poor. J. Gerontol. Ser. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. https://doi.org/10.1093/gerona/gln010 (2009).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Marciniak, R. et al. Effect of meditation on cognitive functions in context of aging and neurodegenerative diseases. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 8, 1–9 (2014).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Mackes, N. K. et al. Early childhood deprivation is associated with alterations in adult brain structure despite subsequent environmental enrichment. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 117, 641–649 (2020).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Gallardo-Gómez, D. et al. Optimal dose and type of exercise to improve cognitive function in older adults: A systematic review and bayesian model-based network meta-analysis of RCTs. Ageing Res. Rev. 76, 101591 (2022).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Bherer, L., Erickson, K. I. & Liu-Ambrose, T. A review of the effects of physical activity and exercise on cognitive and brain functions in older adults. J. Aging Res. https://doi.org/10.1155/2013/657508 (2013).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Agahi, N., Shaw, B. A. & Fors, S. Social and economic conditions in childhood and the progression of functional health problems from midlife into old age. J. Epidemiol. Community Health. https://doi.org/10.1136/jech-2013-203698 (2014).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Phillips, S. P. et al. Life-course social and economic circumstances, gender, and resilience in older adults: The longitudinal International Mobility in Aging Study (IMIAS). SSM Popul. Health. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssmph.2016.09.007 (2016).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Shankar, A. et al. Loneliness, social isolation, and behavioral and biological health indicators in older adults. Health Psychol. 30, 377–385 (2011).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Paúl, C., Ribeiro, O. & Santos, P. Cognitive impairment in old people living in the community. Arch. Gerontol. Geriatr. 51, 121–124 (2010).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Joe, W., Mishra, U. S. & Navaneetham, K. Socio-economic inequalities in child health: Recent evidence from India. Glob. Public Health 5, 493–508 (2010).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Hemalatha, R. et al. Mapping of variations in child stunting, wasting and underweight within the states of India: The Global Burden of Disease Study 2000–2017. EClinicalMedicine. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eclinm.2020.100317 (2020).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Cermakova, P. et al. Socioeconomic position in childhood and cognitive aging in Europe. Neurology 91, E1602–E1610 (2018).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Weng, P.-H. et al. The effect of lifestyle on late-life cognitive change under different socioeconomic status. PLoS ONE 13, e0197676 (2018).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  25. Tucker-Drob, E. M., Johnson, K. E. & Jones, R. N. The cognitive reserve hypothesis: a longitudinal examination of age-associated declines in reasoning and processing speed. Dev. Psychol. 45, 431 (2009).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Craik, Fergus IM, and Timothy A. Salthouse. "Intelligence, Education, and the Brain Reserve Hypothesis: Helen Christensen, Kaarin J. Anstey, Liana S. Leach, and Andrew J. Mackinnon." In The Handbook of Aging and Cognition, pp. 142–197. Psychology Press (2011).

  27. Wang, H.-X. et al. Association of lifelong exposure to cognitive reserve-enhancing factors with dementia risk: A community-based cohort study. PLoS Med. 14, e1002251 (2017).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Gould, F. et al. The effects of child abuse and neglect on cognitive functioning in adulthood. J. Psychiatr. Res. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.01.005 (2012).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Ben-Shlomo, Y. & Kuh, D. A life course approach to chronic disease epidemiology: Conceptual models, empirical challenges and interdisciplinary perspectives. Int. J. Epidemiol. 31, 285–293 (2002).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Kuh, D. et al. A Life Course Approach to Healthy Ageing (OUP Oxford, 2013).

    Book  Google Scholar 

  31. Radford, K. et al. Childhood stress and adversity is associated with late-life dementia in Aboriginal Australians. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 25, 1097–1106 (2017).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Gold, A. L. et al. Are adverse childhood experiences associated with late-life cognitive performance across racial/ethnic groups: Results from the Kaiser Healthy Aging and Diverse Life Experiences study baseline. BMJ Open 11, e042125 (2021).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Cohn-Schwartz, E. & Weinstein, G. Early-life food deprivation and cognitive performance among older Europeans. Maturitas 141, 26–32 (2020).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Zhang, Z., Gu, D. & Hayward, M. D. Childhood nutritional deprivation and cognitive impairment among older Chinese people. Soc. Sci. Med. 71, 941–949 (2010).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Zhang, Z. et al. The long arm of childhood in China: Early-life conditions and cognitive function among middle-aged and older adults. J. Aging Health 30, 1319–1344 (2018).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Kobayashi, L. C. et al. Childhood deprivation and later-life cognitive function in a population-based study of older rural South Africans. Soc. Sci. Med. 190, 20–28 (2017).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Maharani, A. Childhood socioeconomic status and cognitive function later in life: Evidence from a National Survey in Indonesia. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry Neurol. 33, 214–222 (2020).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  38. International Institute for Population Sciences (IIPS), NPHCE, MoHFW, et al. Longitudinal Ageing Study in India (LASI) Wave 1. Mumbai, India, 2020.

  39. Juva, K. et al. Functional assessment scales in detecting dementia. Age Ageing 26, 393–400 (1997).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Blankson AN, McArdle JJ. A brief report on the factor structure of the cognitive measures in the HRS/AHEAD studies. J. Aging Res. 2014, 5. https://doi.org/10.1155/2014/7985142014 (2014).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Saenz, J. L. et al. Household use of polluting cooking fuels and late-life cognitive function: A harmonized analysis of India, Mexico, and China. Environ. Int. 156, 106722 (2021).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Franks, P., Gold, M. R. & Fiscella, K. Sociodemographics, self-rated health, and mortality in the US. Soc. Sci. Med. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0277-9536(02)00281-2 (2003).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Kumar, M., Srivastava, S. & Muhammad, T. Relationship between physical activity and cognitive functioning among older Indian adults. Sci. Rep. 12, 1–13 (2022).

    ADS  Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  44. Muhammad, T., Meher, T. & Sekher, T. V. Association of elder abuse, crime victimhood and perceived neighbourhood safety with major depression among older adults in India: a cross-sectional study using data from the LASI baseline survey (2017–2018). BMJ Open 11, e055625 (2021).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Srivastava, S. et al. Distinguishing between household headship with and without power and its association with subjective well-being among older adults: An analytical cross-sectional study in India. BMC Geriatr. 21, 1–13 (2021).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Sharma, P., Maurya, P. & Muhammad, T. Number of chronic conditions and associated functional limitations among older adults: Cross-sectional findings from the longitudinal aging study in India. BMC Geriatr. 21, 1–12 (2021).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Srivastava, S. & Muhammad, T. Violence and associated health outcomes among older adults in India: A gendered perspective. SSM Popul. Health https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ssmph.2020.100702 (2020).

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Indian Standard Classification of Education | Government of India, Ministry of Education. Accessed 31 March 2022 https://www.education.gov.in/en/indian-standard-classification-education.

  49. Zacharias, A. & Vakulabharanam, V. Caste stratification and wealth inequality in India. World Dev. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.worlddev.2011.04.026 (2011).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Aiken, Leona S., Stephen G. West, and Steven C. Pitts. "Multiple linear regression." In Handbook of psychology. pp: 481–507 (2003).

  51. Webster, A. Heteroscedasticity. Introd. Regres. Anal. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203182567-12 (2020).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Alin, A. Multicollinearity. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Comput. Stat. https://doi.org/10.1002/wics.84 (2010).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. StatCorp. Stata Statistical Software (StataCorp LP).

  54. Tsang, R. S. M., Gallacher, J. E. & Bauermeister, S. The long arm of childhood socioeconomic deprivation on mid- to later-life cognitive trajectories: A cross-cohort analysis. Alzheimer's & Dementia: Diagnosis, Assessment & Disease Monitoring. 14(1), e12322 (2021).

    Google Scholar 

  55. Melrose, R. J. et al. Early life development in a multiethnic sample and the relation to late life cognition. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 70, 519–531 (2014).

    Google Scholar 

  56. Steffener, J. & Stern, Y. Exploring the neural basis of cognitive reserve in aging. BBA Mol. Basis Dis. 1822, 467–473 (2012).

    CAS  Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Greenfield, E. A. & Moorman, S. M. Childhood socioeconomic status and later life cognition: Evidence from the Wisconsin Longitudinal Study. J. Aging Health 31, 1589–1615 (2019).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Hair, N. L. et al. Association of child poverty, brain development, and academic achievement. JAMA Pediatr. 169, 822–829 (2015).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Marmot, M. et al. Relative contribution of early life and adult socioeconomic factors to adult morbidity in the Whitehall II study. J. Epidemiol. Community Health 55, 301–307 (2001).

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Luo, Y. & Waite, L. J. The impact of childhood and adult SES on physical, mental, and cognitive well-being in later life. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 60, 93–101 (2005).

    Google Scholar 

  61. Brayne, C. et al. Education, the brain and dementia: Neuroprotection or compensation?. Brain 133, 2210–2216 (2010).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Foubert-Samier, A. et al. Education, occupation, leisure activities, and brain reserve: A population-based study. Neurobiol. Aging 33(423), e15-423 (2012).

    Google Scholar 

  63. Alley, D., Suthers, K. & Crimmins, E. Education and cognitive decline in older Americans: Results from the AHEAD sample. Res. Aging 29, 73–94 (2007).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Jorm, A. F. et al. Occupation type as a predictor of cognitive decline and dementia in old age. Age Ageing 27, 477–483 (1998).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Wight, R. G. et al. Urban neighborhood context, educational attainment, and cognitive function among older adults. Am. J. Epidemiol. 163, 1071–1078 (2006).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Wu, F. et al. Social-economic status and cognitive performance among Chinese aged 50 years and older. PLoS ONE 11, 1–9 (2016).

    Google Scholar 

  67. Denton, M., Prus, S. & Walters, V. Gender differences in health: A Canadian study of the psychosocial, structural and behavioural determinants of health. Soc. Sci. Med. 58, 2585–2600 (2004).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  68. van de Straat, V. et al. Early predictors of impaired sleep: A study on life course socioeconomic conditions and sleeping problems in older adults. Aging Ment. Health 24, 322–332 (2020).

    PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  69. von Arx, M. et al. The role of adult socioeconomic and relational reserves regarding the effect of childhood misfortune on late-life depressive symptoms. SSM-Popul. health 8, 100434 (2019).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Lyu, J. Gender differences in the association between childhood socioeconomic status and cognitive function in later life. J. Geriatr. 2015, 1–8 (2015).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Kaplan, G. A. et al. Childhood socioeconomic position and cognitive function in adulthood. Int. J. Epidemiol. 30, 256–263 (2001).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Small, S. A. et al. Selective decline in memory function among healthy elderly. Neurology 52, 1392–1392 (1999).

    CAS  PubMed  Article  Google Scholar 

  73. Zhang, Z., Hayward, M. D. & Yu, Y. L. Life course pathways to racial disparities in cognitive impairment among older Americans. J. Health Soc. Behav. 57, 184–199 (2016).

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

  1. Department of Family & Generations, International Institute for Population Sciences, Mumbai, Maharashtra, 400088, India

    T. Muhammad & T. V. Sekher

  2. International Institute for Population Sciences, Mumbai, 400088, India

    Paramita Debnath

  3. Department of Survey Research & Data Analytics, International Institute for Population Sciences, Mumbai, India

    Shobhit Srivastava

Authors

  1. T. Muhammad

    You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar

  2. Paramita Debnath

    You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar

  3. Shobhit Srivastava

    You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar

  4. T. V. Sekher

    You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar

Contributions

Substantial contributions to the conception or design of the work: T.M. and T.V.S.; Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data for the work: S.S. and T.M.; Drafting the work or revising it critically for important intellectual content: T.M., P.D., and S.S.; Final approval of the version to be published: T.M., P.D., S.S. and T.V.S.; Agreement to be accountable for all aspects of the work in ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved: T.M., T.V.S., P.D. and S.S.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Shobhit Srivastava.

Ethics declarations

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.

Reprints and Permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Muhammad, T., Debnath, P., Srivastava, S. et al. Childhood deprivations predict late-life cognitive impairment among older adults in India. Sci Rep 12, 12786 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-16652-y

Download citation

  • Received: 16 August 2021

  • Accepted: 13 July 2022

  • Published: 27 July 2022

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-16652-y

Comments

By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.

What cognitive changes typically occur during adulthood?

With advancing age, healthy adults typically exhibit decreases in performance across many different cognitive abilities such as memory, processing speed, spatial ability, and abstract reasoning.

Which of the following changes occurs during middle adulthood?

Middle adulthood, or middle age, is the time of life between ages 40 and 65. During this time, people experience many physical changes that signal that the person is aging, including gray hair and hair loss, wrinkles and age spots, vision and hearing loss, and weight gain, commonly called the middle age spread.

Which of the following is true of the sense of personal control in middle age?

Which of the following is true of the sense of personal control in middle age? The sense of personal control is frequently challenged by increased responsibility and the aging process.

Which statement best explains the relationship between aging and intelligence during middle adulthood?

Which statement best explains the relationship between aging and intelligence during middle adulthood? Older adults are more likely to forget which items they wanted to buy at a grocery store than they are to forget how to drive a car.